Photo de l'auteur

H. J. Eysenck (1916–1997)

Auteur de Know Your Own I.Q.

119 oeuvres 1,933 utilisateurs 13 critiques 1 Favoris

A propos de l'auteur

Hans J. Eysenck (1916-1997), a professor of psychology at the University of London and the director of its psychological department at the Institute of Psychiatry.
Crédit image: Hans Eysenck

Œuvres de H. J. Eysenck

Know Your Own I.Q. (1962) 316 exemplaires
Check Your Own I.Q. (1900) 311 exemplaires
Uses and Abuses of Psychology (1900) 167 exemplaires
Sense and Nonsense in Psychology (1953) 131 exemplaires
Fact and Fiction in Psychology (1965) 75 exemplaires
Inequality of Man (1973) 25 exemplaires
Sex, violence, and the media (1978) 25 exemplaires
Know Your Own Psi-Q (1983) 22 exemplaires
Crime and Personality (1964) 20 exemplaires
Le prove d'intelligenza (1966) 18 exemplaires
You and Neurosis (1977) 16 exemplaires
The psychology of politics (1998) 13 exemplaires
Dimensions of Personality (1947) 13 exemplaires
Encyclopedia of psychology (1972) 12 exemplaires
Sex and Personality (1976) 10 exemplaires
The Intelligence Controversy (1981) 9 exemplaires
Intelligence: A New Look (1998) 8 exemplaires
Case studies in behaviour therapy (1976) 7 exemplaires
A Textbook of Human Psychology (1976) 7 exemplaires
The Psychology of Sex (1977) 7 exemplaires
The Effects of Psychotherapy (1995) 6 exemplaires
A Model for personality (1981) 6 exemplaires
Ismerd meg az I.Q.-dat! (1995) 6 exemplaires
Psychological basis of ideology (1978) 5 exemplaires
La mente nuda (1985) 5 exemplaires
Tunne oma võimeid 5 exemplaires
A Model for intelligence (1982) 4 exemplaires
Menneskesinnets gåter (1982) 3 exemplaires
Causes and Effects of Smoking (1980) 3 exemplaires
Faça Seu Teste -VOL. 2 (1973) 3 exemplaires
Test zelf uw intelligentie (1990) 3 exemplaires
Psicologia: Hechos y Palabreria (1987) 3 exemplaires
Know Your Child's IQ (1997) 3 exemplaires
Wegwijzer in de psychologie (1982) 2 exemplaires
Psicologia Umana 2 exemplaires
Conosci la tua personalità (1986) 2 exemplaires
Tabaco, Personalidad Y Estres (1994) 2 exemplaires
Psicología del sexo (1981) 2 exemplaires
Tunne oma IQ (2004) 1 exemplaire
Kend dig selv 1 exemplaire
Eysenck on extraversion (1973) 1 exemplaire
How Clever Are You? 1 exemplaire
Dr. Hans J. Eysenck 1 exemplaire
De Eysenck IQ-test (1995) 1 exemplaire
Knjiga inteligencije (1986) 1 exemplaire

Étiqueté

Partage des connaissances

Nom légal
Eysenck, Hans Jürgen
Date de naissance
1916-03-04
Date de décès
1997-09-04
Sexe
male
Nationalité
Germany (birth)
UK (naturalized)
Lieu de naissance
Berlin, German Empire
Lieu du décès
London, England, UK
Études
University of London (PhD, Psychology, 1940)
Professions
psychologist
Relations
Burt, Sir Cyril (supervisor)
Eysenck, Michael (son)
Organisations
Institute of Psychiatry, King's College London
World Cultural Council
Prix et distinctions
William James Fellow Award (1994)
Courte biographie
Obtained his PhD degree in psychology after school and university experience in Germany, France and England. Having worked as a psychologist during the Second World War at the Mill Hill Emergency Hospital, he was appointed Professor of Psychology in the University of London, and Director of the Psychological Department at the Institute of Psychiatry (Maudsley and Bethlem Royal Hospitals). He lectured in many countries, and was Visiting Professor at the Universities of Pennsylvania and of California. Known mainly through his experimental researches in the field of personality, he wrote hundreds of articles in technical journals. He advocated the highest degree of scientific rigour in the design of psychological experiments and was very critical of much loose thinking current during his lifetime under the guise of 'psychology'.

Membres

Critiques

Sinceramente pensavo fosse una cavolata, ma gli ho concesso il beneficio del dubbio e devo dire di essere rimasta sorpresa.
L'unico capitolo che secondo me è del tutto errato è quello riguardante il senso dell'umorismo. Ma noi italiani si sa, abbiamo un senso dell'umorismo che non ha niente a che vedere con quello anglo-americano!
½
 
Signalé
Anshin | Jan 11, 2024 |
Åtta fullständiga intelligenttester med facit och resultattabell.
 
Signalé
CalleFriden | 1 autre critique | Mar 1, 2023 |
 
Signalé
atman2019 | 1 autre critique | Dec 3, 2019 |
Reading this is frankly a struggle. Eysenck's giant ego is on every page. But this is an autobiography, and - as becomes clear - his achievements generally matched his high opinion of himself. The book covers Eysenck's career and controversies (most notably IQ and race) comprehensively, and gives a reasonable airing of all sides of the arguments. It gives a good insight into the development of the 'school' of cognitive behavioral therapy, providing a window into the politics of introducing a new theory into academia and professional practice. Eysenck was the rebel through all of these years, challenging 'accepted practices' and finding himself on the receiving end of a great deal of resentment from vested interests. And this was long before taking on the 'world' with his research into IQ and race.

What comes through strongly with this book is Eysenck's honesty. He writes it 'just as he sees it' and takes the reader with him as he lays out the events and issues from his perspective very clearly. This particularity - making what is not said is just as significant as what is said - gives the reader the opportunity to gain an insight into a few matters that Eysenck appears to have chosen to ignore, or was completely unaware of.

Throughout his career Eysenck championed scientific statistical methods in psychology. His approach took him to the forefront of psychology, but arguably he then took the science too far and too seriously in drawing conclusions about race and IQ. Eysenck could have defended himself in the bitter debate that followed by saying that he was - as a good scientist - simply observing rather than concluding. But it is clear from his writing that it wasn't in his nature to take the cautious approach, particularly as he'd (nearly) always been right in the past in the face of entrenched opposition. There is a sense that the virulence – and illogical arguments – of the opposition only served to re-inforced his belief in the correctness of his position. He had, however, two blind spots

This book, and Eysenck's history, does not suggest that he put any great effort into assuring himself that his audience understood that scientific method involves arriving at a qualified truth through a process incorporating both rigor and humility. The humility in this sense, is the process of putting one's own theories 'through the wringer', looking for every possible explanation as to why they are wrong before releasing them on the world, and even then admitting the possibility that further research might overturn them.

Eysenck's highest ideal, the basis for his immensely high opinion of himself was his thoroughness. This suggests he would not have neglected to go through the process of attempting to tear down his own arguments. But Eysenck's immense pride also suggests that he may have given less attention to making clear, and ensuring that the public understood that he was making clear, that he and his results were bound by the rules of science. That conclusions were 'tentative' and always subject to further revision. One suspects that Eysenck would have acknowledged this if asked, given his commitment to being a capital 'S' scientist. This admission might have taken some of the heat out of the furore over race and IQ that Eysenck's research unleashed. But it was not asked, and Eysenck didn't seem to feel the need to offer the qualification.

It was not in Eysenck's nature to 'reach down' in debate to explain the first principles of the philosophy of science to students and colleagues. One gets the sense in his autobiography that the public criticism of his work on race and IQ did not bother him overmuch. But he was immensely frustrated when students and colleagues failed to use scientific principles in bringing the argument to him. He pointedly includes a photo of graffiti at Birmingham University that read, “Uphold genuine academic freedom. Fascist Eysenck has no right to speak!”.

When his actual science was challenged, Eysenck was able to show that his statistics were completely thorough and correct. The debate, however, very seldom lifted into the realm of validity. That is to say deep questions about the design of the experiments underpinning the theory, what might be concluded from the statistics that are collected, and the development and weighing of alternate hypothesis. This was the area of greatest weakness in Eysenck's work, but also the area largely neglected by his detractors in the public arena, and his critics in the scientific community.

The toxic nature of the public debate tended to overwhelm the content of the scientific debate. Eysenck couldn't, and wasn't inclined to, develop his ideas away from the glare of public execration, with the result that little progress was made in furthering understanding of IQ and race at the time, or since. More cautious scientists than Eysenck (and that is most of them) have drawn lessons from Eysenck's experiences and generally steered clear of making further comment on the issue.

The parallels with the debate about the science of climate change are interesting although not absolute. At the end of the day this is a difficult, but interesting autobiography, and an excellent insight into the development of cognitive behavioral psychology. Furthermore, it is essential reading for gaining an insight into the interaction of science and public opinion.
… (plus d'informations)
½
1 voter
Signalé
nandadevi | Mar 4, 2012 |

Listes

Vous aimerez peut-être aussi

Auteurs associés

Statistiques

Œuvres
119
Membres
1,933
Popularité
#13,324
Évaluation
½ 3.3
Critiques
13
ISBN
319
Langues
15
Favoris
1

Tableaux et graphiques